Many
factors may influence the development of particular floor types. Here we outline
three contextual aspects of the discourse which shape floor development: participants
and their roles within the group; verbal activity (topic and communicative
action); and a selection of medium-related features. The effects of these
are then investigated with reference to examples of the floor types outlined
in part four. These occur within a single stretch of SCMC discourse text.
Both
Edelsky (1981) and Herring (forthcoming) concentrate on gender as key contextual
aspects of floor development. Edelsky is careful to note that the F1 and F2
floor types are gender-independent, though participation by men in F1 floors
was far greater than participation by women (1981:415). Herring concludes
that her findings are of two gender styles rather than two different floor
types: a male style associated with individual power and a female style associated
with accommodation to others (forthcoming:18). In the discourse of the virtual
community in question here, Webheads, the status of the participants
and their various role relations may be more influential than gender in shaping
floor structure. A primary though troublesome distinction is between expert
user teachers of English on one hand, and learners of English on the other.
The term expert user is used in preference to the term native speaker
for the obvious reason that expert users of a language are not necessarily
native speakers. For discussion of this and other issues surrounding the notion
of the ?native speaker? see Rampton (1990). Though there are students and
tutors in the Webheads group, care is taken to minimise any perceived
divide. Nonetheless, the role of participant as learner, as tutor, or as other
interested party (e.g., help-desk volunteer; researcher) is often, though
not always, clear. Incidentally, Webheads ?learners? are sometimes
English language teachers in off-screen life. Another distinction may be made
between the more and the less technologically able, or electronically literate
members of the group, regardless of their level of English. Proficiency in
English does not automatically confer proficiency in the use of the technologies
of CMC, as any first-time visitor to an internet chat room will testify. Thus
a proficient technophile may find him- or herself cast in the role of tutor,
but tutor in the use of the technologies of electronic literacy.
There
is a growing body of research on role relations in virtual communities. See,
for example, Cherny (1999) on life in a MUD community; Turkle (1995) on roles
and identity on-screen, and Smith and Kollock?s (1999) collection of papers
on online communities. On the subject of the roles of language teachers and
students face-to-face and online, see Salmon (2000); Kern (1995); Warschauer
(1996; 1999).
The
research of Shultz et al. (1982) showed that floor patterns were associated
with the speech activity, and that changes in floor patterns occurred when
the speech activity changed. ?Speech activities?, say Shultz et al. (1982:96),
are: ?units of discourse in conversation that are longer than a sentence and
may consist of one discourse topic, or may consist of a set of connected topics
and subtopics.? The term speech activity is from Gumperz (1977) and
is a synthesis of the current communicative action and the broad topic of
the conversation, for example, ?discussing politics?; ?chatting about the
weather? (Gumperz, 1977:206). The communicative action is the name given to
the type of conversation which might be happening at any time, for example
chatting, explaining, discussing, or arguing.
Shultz
et al., when making the important link between conversational floor
patterns and speech activity, found that certain types of speech activity
often corresponded with certain types of floor. That is to say, when the speech
activity was ?chatting about how much everything costs in the stores nowadays?,
the appropriate floor was a multiple conversational floor with overlapping
speech. And when the speech activity was ?explaining why and where the father?is
going out of town?? there is only one floor, where the parents are the primary
speakers (Shultz et al, 1982: 97). Because SCMC is written rather than
spoken, the term verbal activity is used here in preference to speech
activity.
The
floor, then, is not defined by topic, or aboutness, alone. This
is partly, but not entirely, because topics and their boundaries themselves
are such difficult things to identify. As Brown and Yule do, we can
consider topic as ?what is being talked about?(1983:71). In addition, Brown
and Yule explain that within a broad topic framework there are elements of
personal ?speaker?s topics?. By considering speaker?s topic, they recognise
that within a particular framework where the general topic may be generally
or loosely agreed, the individual participants sometimes have differing views
on what the topic is or where the focus should be. When investigating speaker?s
topic, text of discourse is analysed: ?? not in terms of how we would characterise
the participants? shared information, but in terms of a process in which each
participant expresses a personal topic within the general topic framework
as a whole? (1983:88).
Topics frequently drift; that is, they move gradually from one area into others, without an easily discernible topic boundary. Topic drift, or shading, as a feature of spoken conversation has been commented on by Hobbs (1990), Schegloff and Sacks (1973) and Jefferson (1984) among others. In SCMC rapid topic drift - or topic decay - is the subject of work by Herring and Nix (1997).
The broad
topic itself as it relates to focus of attention is a concern here. In the
Webheads SCMC sessions, it is common for a learner to explicitly raise
a language learning point. When the topic of the discourse is so obviously
related to the acquisition of the L2 (English), it is expected that attention
would be focused towards that floor of conversation. It is also common for
a participant (and no distinction is made here between learners and tutors)
to discuss an aspect of the technologies of SCMC. Particular floor patterns
are associated with topics relating to the development of second or foreign
language skills and with topics relating to the development of electronic
literacy skills. This is demonstrated and explained in the analysis and discussion
below.
There
are also medium-related reasons for particular floors to develop in SCMC.
In particular, the emergence of the multiple conversational floor may be associated
with the way in which a written conversation occurs. Cherny (1999:180) maintains
that: ?Multiple participant floors are in fact easier to achieve [in SCMC
discourse] than they are in face-to-face conversations.? She claims this is
due to the lack of overlap (i.e. the inability to co-construct turns)
in the medium. We might also note that the ability to scroll up and re-read
previous turns, coupled with the slower speed of the unfolding discourse compared
to spoken conversation, facilitate the emergence of multiple floors, and enable
an individual to participate in a number of floors simultaneously.
Topics in SCMC are prone to recur, leading to the re-emergence of particular floor types. This is the case when participants are carrying out more than one on-screen activity. That is to say, when they are multitasking. At certain points in the discourse something happens in another space on the internet which is relevant to a previous topic which then over-rides the current topics. The floor type may consequently revert to a previous one.
This
discussion is based on a stretch of SCMC discourse text of 36 turns in length,
presented below as example 12:
1 |
VanceS
says, "I never knew what chili was exactly before" |
2 |
BJB
. o O ( that will open a web window to go with your text client ) |
3 |
LianA
says, "come to china, then you will know what it is, vance." |
4 |
BJB
. o O ( I hope ) |
5 |
LianA
oO |
6 |
PhilB
says, "Vance, there's a lot of confusion between the words "chili"
and "chile" (borrowed from Spanish." |
7 |
Sue
[guest] asks, "Lian, how much did you take on GRE?" |
8 |
LianA
asks, "what does burn the scandle from the two ends mean? who can
help?" |
9 |
LianA
says, "not very high, only 2160" |
10 |
BJB
[to Lian]: "how long do you think a candle will last if you burn
both ends?" |
11 |
LianA
says, "nol not candle but scandle" |
12 |
LianA
says, "no---typo" |
13 |
Sue
[guest] says, "so hight? i am wondering i can only take 1500" |
14 |
PhilB
says, "Lian, it's a play on words." |
15 |
VanceS
says, "I've been to China several times, but never to Wuhan" |
16 |
LianA
says, "it said if you burn the scandle from 2 ends, you will be
a busy man." |
17 |
BJB
thinks there are several threads to this conversation |
18 |
VanceS
says, "Also you will burn yourself out" |
19 |
LianA
says, "welcome vance to wuhan next time to china." |
20 |
PhilB
says, "Normally to "burn the candle on both ends" means
to work so much you tire yourself out. With "scandal" instead
of "candle" it sounds like Bill Clinton with his hot interns.
<g>" |
21 |
BJB
chuckles. Same result, though. |
22 |
VanceS
. o O ( this is a normal consequence of multitasking ) |
23 |
VanceS
says, "he must have had too many hot interns in the fire" |
24 |
LianA
giggles |
25 |
PhilB
asks, "Hey, I found a new free resource called "stuffincommon
virtual communities". Anyone heard of it?" |
26 |
VanceS
says, "never" |
27 |
Sue
[guest] says, "no" |
28 |
LianA
says, "no" |
29 |
PhilB
asks, "Wanna see?" |
30 |
VanceS
says, "sure" |
31 |
Sue
[guest] says, "sure" |
32 |
PhilB
says, "It has chat, tools, and a neat whiteboard." |
33 |
LianA
says, "yes." |
34 |
PhilB
asks, "I'm going to project. Sue, Lian, do you know about projections?" |
35 |
LianA
says, "yes" |
36 |
Sue
[guest] says, "not sure" |
Example 12
There
are three distinct phases to this stretch of SCMC text:
Turns
1-22: a period where a number of conversations continue simultaneously (a
multiple conversational floor);
Turns
8-24: a period where there is one main conversation where many participants
hold the floor (a collaborative floor);
Turns
25-36: a period where one participant is the floor holder, supported by others
(a speaker-and-supporter floor).
It
will be immediately noted that the first and second phases overlap considerably,
while there is a clear boundary between the second and third phases. Floor
boundaries in SCMC are not necessarily distinct. On this occasion, a collaborative
floor is the main floor in a multiple conversational floor; when the other
conversations in the multiple floor are completed, it becomes briefly the
only floor in a single collaborative floor. Here the multiple floor continues
from turn 1 to turn 22. The floor which emerges at turn 8 becomes the main
floor. At turn 23 it becomes the only floor, as previous conversations are
completed. At turn 25 the pattern shifts decisively to a single speaker-and-supporter
floor.
An
analytical technique for discussing floors in SCMC is to isolate individual
conversations from the text. Naturally, the objection to this might be: how
can we know post hoc and without being informed by the participants
which turns belong to which conversation? The answer must be
that we cannot be certain. Nonetheless, despite the possibility of there being
other interpretations, it seems quite clear that all but one turn (turn 5)
can be accounted for in the way described below.
In
examples 12a to 12f the individual floors and elements of floors in the stretch
of SCMC discourse presented above as example 12 are discussed with reference
to the features which can be said to influence floor development. Before we
turn to these isolated sections, there are three points to note. Firstly,
this stretch of discourse text is not a complete textual record of the interaction.
The log was originally recorded by VanceS, and begins 40 turns after his arrival
at Tapped In. However, the other participants had already commenced
the interaction. Thus some of the conversations in the example are incomplete;
either because they had started before the extract begins, or because they
continue after it ends. The example contains no instances of a participant
entering or leaving the conversation. An illuminating study of openings in
SCMC can be found in Rintel, Mulholland, and Pittam (2001). Secondly, it should
be recalled that the disrupted turn adjacency inherent in the medium gives
a certain arbitrariness to the position of the individual turns in the text
in relation to the other turns. Thirdly, we should also briefly note some
relevant contextual details about the participants:
VanceS is the founder tutor of the Webheads group.
BJB works as a volunteer on the helpdesk at Tapped In.
PhilB is an English teacher who coordinates another group at Tapped In.
LianA and Sue are English language learners with Webheads.
In
example 12a, the turns of Vance, LianA and PhilB belong to the end of the
same conversation, a collaborative floor within a multiple conversational
floor which has the verbal activity chatting about chilli and China.
The discussion about chilli had been continuing for a number of turns before
the beginning of this extract.
1 |
VanceS
says, "I never knew what chili was exactly before" |
|
|
3 |
LianA
says, "come to china, then you will know what it is, vance." |
|
|
6 |
PhilB
says, "Vance, there's a lot of confusion between the words "chili"
and "chile" (borrowed from Spanish." |
|
|
15 |
VanceS
says, "I've been to China several times, but never to Wuhan" |
Example 12a
Contextual and temporal aspects of the discourse would suggest that much attention is paid to personal speaker?s topic (writer?s topic?) in SCMC. In example 12a above, within a broad topic framework which could be said to be about chilli, LianA?s speaker?s topic is China. This also becomes Vance?s speaker?s topic in turn 15; a topic drift has taken place within a floor of conversation. Neither the topic of chilli or of China are developed any further in the interaction.
In Example 12b we see the end of another conversation. BJB has been explaining to Sue how to open the graphical interface of Tapped In:
2 |
BJB
. o O ( that will open a web window to go with your text client ) |
|
|
4 |
BJB
. o O ( I hope ) |
Example 12b
We
note that BJB is using a device whereby her turn is displayed inside an ASCII
?thinks? bubble. This is done in Tapped In by prefacing the turn with
the command ?/thinks?. We might infer that she uses this technique because
the turns are directed towards only one among many participants. It is possible
in Tapped In to send a turn privately to another participant using
the ?/whisper? command. That BJB does not do this suggests, in the light of
her role with Tapped In, that she feels the information might be of
use to more than one participant.
Example
12c is an exchange of three turns spread over seven turns of the extract.
7 |
Sue
[guest] asks, "Lian, how much did you take on GRE?" |
|
|
9 |
LianA
says, "not very high, only 2160" |
|
|
13 |
Sue
[guest] says, "so hight? i am wondering i can only take 1500" |
Example 12c
The
two language learners here are discussing an English language test. Although
not proficient in English, they are both adept at SCMC discourse. Both Sue
and LianA participate in more than one conversation in this extract; turns
by LianA appear in four of the six isolated examples highlighted here.
Example
12d is an aside:
17 |
BJB
thinks there are several threads to this conversation |
|
|
22 |
VanceS
. o O ( this is a normal consequence of multitasking ) |
Example 12d
The topic
here is the conversation itself. In spoken discourse the turns would be expected
to appear together, as an adjacency pair or as the initiation and response
of an exchange. However, here in SCMC they are separated by four unrelated
turns. Also of note is the fact that BJB?s turn is posted in the third person
as an emote or metacomment; that is, a comment on the unfolding conversation.
Vance?s turn is also a representation of something other than speech, using
as BJB did earlier the cartoon ?thinks? bubble.
Example
12e is the main floor of the multiple floor. The previous examples (12a to
12d) can be considered side floors, or even mere asides, in the multiple floor.
8 |
LianA
asks, "what does burn the scandle from the two ends mean? who can
help?" |
|
|
10 |
BJB
[to Lian]: "how long do you think a candle will last if you burn
both ends?" |
11 |
LianA
says, "nol not candle but scandle" |
12 |
LianA
says, "no---typo" |
|
|
14 |
PhilB
says, "Lian, it's a play on words." |
|
|
16 |
LianA
says, "it said if you burn the scandle from 2 ends, you will be
a busy man." |
|
|
18 |
VanceS
says, "Also you will burn yourself out" |
|
|
20 |
PhilB
says, "Normally to "burn the candle on both ends" means
to work so much you tire yourself out. With "scandal" instead
of "candle" it sounds like Bill Clinton with his hot interns.
<g>" |
21 |
BJB
chuckles. Same result, though. |
|
|
23 |
VanceS
says, "he must have had too many hot interns in the fire" |
24 |
LianA
giggles |
Example 12e
This is a collaborative floor in that four participants are involved in its development. The contention is that it dominates because the verbal activity is explaining about a phrase LianA has read, which involves the communicative action explaining. The topic, raised quite explicitly by LianA in turn 8, is a phrase that LianA has presumably read or heard and that she wants help in understanding what it means. As noted above, LianA is an English language learner, and Webheads is a virtual community dedicated to language learning. In other words, when a language learner raises a language learning point, much of the focus redirects towards that particular floor, the floor becomes collaborative, and the communicative action of the verbal activity orients towards ?explaining?.
In
example 12f the floor type can also be attributed directly to the participant
and the verbal activity:
25 |
PhilB
asks, "Hey, I found a new free resource called "stuffincommon
virtual communities". Anyone heard of it?" |
26 |
VanceS
says, "never" |
27 |
Sue
[guest] says, "no" |
28 |
LianA
says, "no" |
29 |
PhilB
asks, "Wanna see?" |
30 |
VanceS
says, "sure" |
31 |
Sue
[guest] says, "sure" |
32 |
PhilB
says, "It has chat, tools, and a neat whiteboard." |
33 |
LianA
says, "yes." |
34 |
PhilB
asks, "I'm going to project. Sue, Lian, do you know about projections?" |
35 |
LianA
says, "yes" |
36 |
Sue
[guest] says, "not sure" |
Example 12f
This
is a single floor with one floor holder being supported by other participants.
In the terminology adopted here, from Hayashi (1991) and Cherny (1999), it
is a speaker-and-supporter floor. The communicative action of the verbal activity
is primarily didactic: PhilB is demonstrating an internet resource called
?stuffincommon?. There is a sub-topic in turns 34 to 36: using the project
command in Tapped In. The communicative activity remains explanatory.
In
this section the concern has been with a limited set of patterns of participation
and floor types. There are undoubtedly many other patterns which relate to
the development of other floor types. This notwithstanding, the conclusion
can be drawn from this analysis that floor development is related to verbal
activity. When the topic of the verbal activity is a language point raised
by a learner, the floor becomes collaborative. It either develops as the single
collaboratively constructed floor or as the main floor of a multiple conversational
floor. When the topic is related to the technologies of electronic literacy
(for example, how to build a website; or where a particular resource can be
found), the floor develops into a speaker-and-supporter floor. The participant
role is also important. In each case the communicative action (explaining/demonstrating)
is pedagogic. However, when the verbal activity is directly related to the
acquisition of the second or foreign language (English), a number of participants
contribute substantive turns. When the verbal activity is related to the development
of the skills of electronic literacy, other participants focus their attention
on the single floor holder.